On Party Identification in Texas
I mentioned in a previous post that I would have more to say about partisan leaners in a future post. Well, this is the post.
First, let’s agree on what
party identification is because there are several competing definitions. I am a
traditionalist, perceiving party identification (PID) as a psychological
attachment to a political party. This definition was originally offered in The American Voter by Professors
Campbell, Converse, Miller, and Stokes at the University of Michigan, which was published in 1960. They maintained that this is one of the
stronger political attitudes, which means that it is less likely to change than
other attitudes. They also maintained that it was usually adopted from one’s
family. Because of its strength and relative permanence, they argued that it is
a filter through which other political attitudes have to pass. They referred to
a “funnel of causality,” tracing a person’s other political attitudes to one’s
partisan identification. Voting behavior, for example, is considered to be
strongly influenced by one’s partisan identification.
The also developed a
method of determining a person’s PID. They asked a basic question: “In
politics, do you consider yourself a Democrat, Republican, or Independent?” For
those responding either Democrat or Republican, they asked a second question: “Are
you a strong or not-so-strong Republican (Democrat)?” For independents, the
second question was: “Are you a pure independent, or do you lean toward one of
the parties?” The result is a seven-point scale, ranging from strong Democrat
on the left to strong Republican on the right. The diagram below illustrates
the scale.
SD WD DLI
I RLI
WR SR
Where do registered voters
in Texas fall on the PID continuum? In other words, what percentage
of Texans falls into each of the seven categories? The results of a May 2012
UT/Texas Tribune poll are represented
in the chart below:
If you consider partisans as strong and weak identifiers
(SD, WD, SR, WR), then 36 percent of Texas
registered voters are Republicans, and 33 percent are Democrats. Independents
make up 30 percent of Texas
registered voters. However, political scientists have argued about where the
“leaners”—Democratic and Republican leaning independents—should be placed.
Since the publication of Bruce Keith et al.’s The Myth of the Independent Voter in 1992, political scientists
have generally agreed that “leaners” are “closet partisans,” who behave like
partisans even though they don’t claim an allegiance to one of the political
parties. I have always found accepting this view difficult. If party
identification is an emotional attachment to a political party and a person
claims no such attachment, why should they be considered partisans? The fact
that “leaners” are more supportive of a party’s positions on issues than weak
partisans and are more likely to vote for the party’s nominees for public
office than weak partisans does not make them partisans.
To some people, the distinction may not seem important, but
it has consequences for campaigns and how campaigns target potential voters. If
leaners are really partisans, then only about 10 percent of the Texas
electorate is independent. If leaners are independents, then about 30 percent
of the Texas electorate is
independent. If it’s only 10 percent, then they aren’t as consequential to
election outcomes, and campaigns should concentrate on mobilizing partisans
with partisan appeals. If however, it’s 30 percent, then the campaigns must
appeal to independents with messages that are different from their partisan
appeals. In other words, it matters!
So, what’s the basis for each side’s view? For those who
consider leaners as “closet partisans”—Keith et al., John Petrocik, Alan Abramowitz,
and others—the principal reason is that leaners vote for the party’s candidate
at a higher rate than weak partisans. Thus, leaners’ behavior (voting) is more
like partisans than like independents. However, as Samuel Abrams and Morris Fiorina
(2011) assert, the causal relationship could be the other direction: voting
causes independents to declare themselves as leaners as they recall their most
recent voting decision. In other words, I voted for Obama in the 2008
presidential election; so I must lean toward the Democratic Party. Or, I voted
for McCain in the 2008 presidential election; so I must lean toward the
Republican Party. Philip Shively made this point in 1980, and this is what I
have always believed. Keith et al. and Petrocik also maintain that leaners’
voting decisions demonstrate temporal consistency so that leaners do not vote
for one party’s candidates in one election and then switch to another party’s
candidates in a subsequent election. Leaners are also no more likely than weak partisans
to split their voting decisions during a particular election, voting for some
Republicans and some Democrats in the same election.
For those who question the lumping of leaners into one of
the partisan camps—Abrams, Fiorina, Todd Eberly, and others—the leaners are not
partisans, challenging the now-accepted view that they are partisans. Most
recently (March 2012), Third Way
released a report by St. Mary's College of Maryland Professor Todd Eberly, using data from a panel
study of three successive elections, that made two points about the leaners: (1)
leaners were significantly more likely to change their party identification
than were Democrats or Republicans, whether strong or weak partisans. (2)
leaners switched their vote choice over elections, and Republican leaners were
more loyal than Democratic leaners. In response to this study, Abramowitz
countered that the study used by Eberly had a small sample size and that there
was a larger panel study, conducted during the 2008-2009 period, which showed
different results. This survey indicated that very high percentages of Democrats (82
percent) and Republicans (73 percent) retained their position as party leaners
from January 2008 until a date more than a year and one-half later. Also,
during this period, only five percent of leaners switched to the opposing
party. He also points out that the leaners voted for the party toward which
they leaned and attributed it to their sharing the dominant ideological
orientation of the party toward which they leaned. In response to Abramowitz,
Eberly noted that the 2009-2009 panel does allow one to observe partisanship
over several elections, unlike the 2000-2004 panel. He added results from a
1992-1997 panel survey. The results of his investigation led Eberly to conclude
that partisan loyalty declines over time and is weakest among weak partisans.
But he also observed that most defections were by leaners. He concludes that about 20
percent of the electorate are not loyal partisans, and that, “in an era of
closely matched political parties and relatively narrow two-party vote shares,
winning and maintaining the support of that 20 percent is crucial.”
So, what do you think? Should leaners be considered
partisans or are they susceptible to appeals from either political party?
Should campaigns concentrate their efforts not only on turning out faithful
partisans but also on winning the independents, whether leaners or pure
independents?
Comments
Post a Comment